Next: 6.2 Biological mechanisms underlying
Up: 6 Discussion and Future
Previous: 6 Discussion and Future
Even though the RF-LISSOM model was not developed as an explanation
for the tilt aftereffect, it exhibits tilt aftereffects that have
nearly all of the features of those measured in humans. With the
appropriate extensions, it is expected to account for all of the known
data on the TAE. The features of the TAE which have already been
demonstrated in the model include those in the following list. Except
where noted, all of these features have been replicated by a number of
researchers and are quite consistent across different studies, so any
viable model of the tilt aftereffect would be expected to account for
them.
- Null at training angle:
- Adaptation does not change the
perceived orientation of the stimulus used during adaptation
(figure 5.2; (Gibson and Radner, 1937).
- Direct effect:
- Similar orientations are misperceived by human
subjects as having a larger difference than they actually do
(Gibson and Radner, 1937), peaking between 5° and 20°,
typically 10°- 13°
([p.216]howard:hso66; Campbell and Maffei 1971; Mitchell and Muir 1976).
In RF-LISSOM, peaks at locations between 5° and 15° have been
observed; 10° is typical (figure 5.2).
- Null between direct and indirect effects:
- Human aftereffects
return to zero somewhere between 25° and
50° (Campbell and Maffei, 1971; Mitchell and Muir, 1976; Muir and Over, 1970).
Zero-crossings between 30° and 60° have been observed
in RF-LISSOM; 45° is typical (figure 5.2).
- Indirect effect:
- Distant orientations are misperceived as
having a smaller difference than they actually do
(Gibson and Radner, 1937), peaking somewhere between 60° and
85° (Campbell and Maffei, 1971; Mitchell and Muir, 1976; Muir and Over, 1970).
In RF-LISSOM, indirect effect peaks between 45° and 75° have
been observed; 60° is typical. These effects vary significantly between studies and different
individuals, as described in
section 6.2.4.
- Time course:
- The TAE magnitude increases at a diminishing rate
with further adaptation (figure 5.10;
Gibson and Radner 1937; Greenlee and Magnussen 1987; Magnussen and Johnsen 1986).
- Orientation-independence:
- The TAE versus angle curve is similar
for all pairs of test and adaptation angles which differ by the same
amount, regardless of the absolute orientation on the retina (Mitchell and Muir, 1976).
Conflicting results were found by previous researchers, who could
not demonstrate effects on oblique lines of adaptation to vertical
lines. This discrepancy has been satisfactorily explained as a
methodological problem of the earlier studies, and the assumption of
orientation independence now seems well established.
- Spatial localization:
- The TAE is localized to the area of the
retina which was trained (Gibson and Radner, 1937). Adaptation for a
figure in one location has no measurable effect on test figures in
other locations sufficiently distant. In the model, this occurs
because weights are only adapted between active neurons, and a
small stimulus activates only neurons in a small cortical area.
Several other aspects of the TAE will likely be exhibited by an
RF-LISSOM model trained on more realistic input distributions. These
simulations do not necessarily require any significant extensions to
the model, but most will require larger cortex sizes and longer
training times. When sufficient computing power is available and the
model is extended as described, RF-LISSOM is expected to account for
these experimental observations as well:
- Higher variance at oblique orientations:
- The TAE versus angle
curve shows greater variance for oblique testing orientations
(Mitchell and Muir, 1976), often sufficiently high to entirely mask the
effect (Campbell and Maffei, 1971). The variance may result from
the smaller number of detectors in the fovea subserving angles that
are neither horizontal nor vertical
(Bauer et al., 1991; Mansfield, 1974). Assuming the
orientation is perceived with a mechanism similar to that in
section 4.5, when fewer detectors are
activated the response will vary more because the average is
being computed from fewer neurons. An RF-LISSOM model trained on a
non-uniform training distribution with a preponderance of horizontal
and vertical lines develops more detectors for horizontal and
vertical orientations.
This type of adaptation has also been observed in kittens
raised in deprived visual environments (Blakemore and van Sluyters, 1975).
Such an anisotropic distribution is probably typical of the early
visual experience of humans (Mansfield, 1974).
- Frequency localization:
- The TAE is selective for spatial
frequency (Ware and Mitchell, 1974); adapting to a figure with narrow bars
has no measurable effect upon a figure with wide bars, and vice
versa. Spatial frequency selectivity has previously been
demonstrated in the RF-LISSOM model
(Miikkulainen et al., 1997; Sirosh et al., 1996). A unified
orientation/spatial
frequency RF-LISSOM model, obtained by training on oriented
Gaussians of different sizes, would exhibit frequency localization
for the same reason as for spatial localization. That is, only a
small range of frequency detectors will be activated by a given
stimulus, so only that range will show adaptation effects.
- Movement direction specificity:
- The TAE is selective for the
direction of movement (Carney, 1982). Adapting on a pattern
moving in one direction past a fixation point does not affect the
orientation judgment of a pattern moving in the opposite direction.
A unified orientation/movement direction RF-LISSOM model, obtained
by training on oriented Gaussians moving in different directions,
would exhibit this property as well. However, simulations have
not yet been performed with moving stimuli for RF-LISSOM, and further
work will be needed to determine how motion should be represented in
the model.
- Ocular transfer:
- The TAE transfers completely from one eye to
the other (Campbell and Maffei, 1971; Gibson and Radner, 1937); adapting one eye
causes equal effects upon test lines in the same location in the
visual field for either eye. A unified RF-LISSOM orientation/ocular
dominance model could be obtained by training on oriented Gaussians
at slightly offset positions in the two eyes. Such a model would
exhibit ocular transfer if the neurons most selective for
orientation were also binocular. If this does not turn out to be
true for the model, it might be that the most plastic neurons in the
cortex are also more likely to be binocular. The latter appears to
be true for human cortex, with the input layer (layer IV) showing
strong ocular dominance but lower adult plasticity, and the output
layers (II, III, V, and VI) showing strong binocularity and high
plasticity ([pp.139-140]daw:visdevel95;
Shatz and Stryker 1978). Modeling such layer-dependent effects
would require a significant extension to RF-LISSOM because currently
all the neurons in each column are grouped together for simulation.
Large-scale combined simulations allowing the study of some of these
features were proposed by Sirosh (1995), but they generally
require greater cortex sizes and training times to represent multiple
dimensions on the same map. Current computer resources are not
sufficient, but with forthcoming advances in technology they should
soon be more practical.
Besides the above, there are a small number of features of the TAE not
yet demonstrated by the current model, and not expected to be found
unless extensions are made:
- Saturation:
- The TAE saturates at approximately 4°
(Campbell and Maffei, 1971; Greenlee and Magnussen, 1987; Magnussen and Johnsen, 1986; Mitchell and Muir, 1976)
but in the RF-LISSOM model it will steadily increase up to at least
20° with sufficiently long adaptation times. Possible mechanisms
for this limit to plasticity will be discussed further in
section 6.2. It is a simple matter to
artificially limit the plasticity allowed in the model, once it is
known what the limiting factor should be.
- Dark recovery:
- After the adaptation stimulus is removed, TAE
magnitude gradually reduces in strength, even in the absence of
visual input. In contrast, the RF-LISSOM model will ordinarily
remain static until further input is received. Explanations for
dark recovery in humans will be discussed further in
section 6.2.3; adults may have
connections which change strength rapidly but temporarily.
There are also some small effects due to the absolute orientation of
the head with respect to gravity; these are presumably due to the
vestibular system, not to processing in V1
(Howard and Templeton, 1966; Wolfe and Held, 1982). Apart from such data not expected
to apply to V1, there is no known psychophysical evidence against the
RF-LISSOM explanation of tilt aftereffects. The following section
will examine the types of cellular processes that may underlie the
psychological effects above, if the TAE is occurring in humans in the
same way it does in the model.
Next: 6.2 Biological mechanisms underlying
Up: 6 Discussion and Future
Previous: 6 Discussion and Future
James A. Bednar
9/19/1997